How do you
imagine our memories work? Are there a thousand file cabinets in our heads,
each dedicated to a different topic? Are there little minions running around organizing and re-organizing our stores of knowledge to incorporate
all the things we learn every day? How come I can remember the atomic mass of
Oxygen but I can’t remember the capital of Connecticut?
The truth is we
still don’t really know. There are several theories about how we store
knowledge but for the most part, it’s still up in the air. However, leaps and
bounds have been made in the past 20 years to elucidate some of the mechanisms
behind acquiring new knowledge. That is, scientists now think they know at
least one way that we humans learn new things.
The process is
called Long-Term Potentiation and it is facilitated by events that occur at the
synapse—the tiny space between two neurons where molecular signals govern
cellular change. There is a molecule called the NMDA-receptor at some of your
synapses that plays a role in modulating neurons’ responses to stimuli. The
NMDA-receptor is like a little gate on the surface of a neuron that is
receiving a signal. Depending on its state (open or closed) the signal from the
first neuron will elicit a strong or weak response from the second neuron. If
the receptor is “open,” special signaling molecules can go through it into the second
neuron which starts responding. So far this sounds pretty normal, an open gate
means more signal can get through and there will be more response. But how does
that help me learn my vocabulary words?
Whether the
NMDA-receptor is open or closed depends on two things: the amount of signal
from the first neuron AND the amount of response from the second neuron. Not
only will more signal from the first neuron open more NMDA-receptor channels
but more response from the second neuron will also open more NMDA-receptor
channels. Furthermore, the more intense
the signal-response event and the more often it occurs, the stronger the same
signal-response event will be next time. The physiological change occurring at
the molecular level strengthens the connection between the two neurons, which
in turn strengthens the bit of knowledge associated with those neurons. This is
precisely why the more times I copy a word and its definition, the more likely
I am to remember it next week. I know you thought
your fourth-grade teacher was just giving you busy-work, but that busy-work may
have actually been helping you commit new facts to memory.
So next time
you’re tempted to skip a few homework problems think about your neurons. You
may not feel it but every problem you do may make that connection between one
neuron and the next a little bit stronger and your NMDA-receptors will thank
you. In fact, I’m going to think about the capital of Connecticut for the rest
of the day. Hartford…Hartford…Hartford……
Sources and further reading
Bliss, T. V. P., Collingridge, G. L. (1993). A synaptic model
of memory: long-term potentiation in the hippocampus. Nature 361, 31-39.
Kikusui, Takefumi, Aoyagi,
Atsushi, Kaneko, Tsugio (2000). Spatial Working Memory is Independent of
Hippocampal CA1 Long-Term Potentiation in Rats. Behavioral
Neuroscience 114, 700-706.
Tsien,
Joe Z., Huerta, Patricio T., Tonegawa, Susumu (1996). The Essential Role of Hipposcampal CA1 NMDA
Receptor-Dependent Synaptic Plasticity in Spatial Memory. Cell 87, 1327-1338.
Comments
Post a Comment